Opening
宋玥 Yue Song Developmental Psychology, Utrecht University
Shuyang Dong Utrecht University
彭聪 Cong Peng Tilburg University
何津 Jin He Vrije University Amsterdam
The best time to talk with speakers and attendees
谭莹莹 Yingying Tan Max Planck Institute for Psycholinguistics
王本驰 Benchi Wang VU
于淋 Lin Yu TBD
丁致中 ChihChung Ting University van Amsterdam
魏永斌 Yongbin Wei Complex Traits Genetics, CNCR, VU Amsterdam
Jian Liang Utrecht University
王雨薇 Yuwei Wang Radboud Institute for Health Sciences
耿海洋 Haiyang Geng University Medical Center Groningen
The best time to talk with speakers and attendees
左西年 Xinian Zuo 中科院心理所
FAQ
Closing
The best time to talk with speakers and attendees
Opening
Closing
FAQ
CAPBS 2018 Meeting will be held 20-21th of October in ancient city of Nijmegen, Netherlands. If you want to be part of this great event as a speaker you knocked at the good door.
View presentationThe origin of prosocial behavior has long been debated. Over the past decade, evidence that toddlers display more happiness when sharing rather than receiving treats has been used to support the argument that humans, by nature, are prosocial. However, sharing is rare and inconsistent at this age, implying that the emotional rewards of sharing might have their origins or be affected by social factors, such as praise. In addition, whether this “warm glow” is specific for sharing behavior, or also the case for other prosocial behaviors (e.g., helping and comforting) is unclear. By replicating (using the same task) and extending previous studies (varying the number of resources and examining praise), 113 Dutch toddlers (M = 21.64 months, SD = 3.33 months, 59 boys) participated in a sharing task in which they received 8, 4 or 2 treats, followed by instrumental helping and comforting tasks (for the latter two tasks the toddlers were praised if they helped or comforted). Toddlers were videotaped during the experiment and happiness later coded. In total, 79 toddlers at least shared, helped or comforted once in the experiment. For these children, results showed that: (1) for sharing, consistent with previous findings, toddlers display more happiness when giving than receiving treats, although the number of resources did not affect toddlers’ happiness. (2) for instrumental helping, toddlers displayed more happiness after helping, and their happiness did not increase after been rewarded (praised). (3) for comforting, toddlers displayed less happiness after witnessing experimenter’s emotional distress (feeling cold), but not more happiness after helping or being praised. Taken together, these results indicate that both sharing and instrumental helping are emotionally rewarding independent of praise, supporting a biological origin of these behaviors.
宋玥 Yue Song PhD Student, Developmental Psychology, Utrecht University
TBD
Money is not appreciated as a gift, partly because money lacks social meanings that express care to the receiver. We tested an idea that money can turn to be acceptable when its amount signals a social meaning. Specifically, Study 1 showed that Chinese appreciate 520RMB cash more than both 500RMB and 550RMB as a Valentine’s Day gift, when 520 means I Love You in Chinese. In Study 2 we found that Chinese receivers chose 520RMB over 530RMB as a birthday gift. This preference for a lower but meaningful amount is particularly salient when it comes from close others.
彭聪 Cong Peng PhD Student, Tilburg University
TBD.
魏永斌 Yongbin Wei PhD Student, Complex Traits Genetics, CNCR, VU Amsterdam
Since December 2015, I'm a PhD student in the Dutch Connectome Lab. I obtained my bachelor degree in Electronic and Information Engineering at Beihang University (former BUAA) and my master degree in Computer Science and Application at Beijing Normal University. During the master program, I did some work on the brain functional connectome and became interested in exploring the mysterious world in our brains.
Now I mainly focus on exploring new analytical methods for neuroimaging data and connectomics. Based on my technical background, I hope to make contributions to unravel neural mechanisms by studying macroscale structural and functional connectivity. I also hope to develop new methods that promote better understanding of the pathology in psychiatric disease and could aid the development of potential biomarkers.
Emotion processing and executive function serve as two crucial cognitive aspects, they interact and influence each other like the south and north pole in the Tai Chi framework. However, it remains largely unknown about what dynamic brain networks support flexible emotion processing and executive function such as working memory, especially interaction between them. Separate activation in amygdala and prefrontal cortex have been long thought to contribute to these processes. However, emerging dynamic brain network studies have begun to examine how dynamics of brain networks support flexible emotion processing, executive function and interaction between them. Focusing on this central issue, in several imaging studies, we used the ‘state of the art’ resting- and task-state fMRI connectivity and network-based approaches, seeking to build a dynamic brain network model of how emotion is processed and how emotion impacts cognitive computations in working memory. Our series of results indicate that dynamic interaction between the salience network (SN), the executive control network (ECN), the default mode network (DMN) and sensory networks enables dynamic representation of emotion (i.e. anxiety and stress) and supports flexible cognitive computation in WM and their dynamic interaction. Previous influential studies suggest that neural oscillation across multiple bands and locus coeruleus (LC)-norepinephrine (NE) neuromodulator serve as potential mechanisms underlying dynamic signal synergy among brain networks, further enable flexible interaction between emotion processing and executive function. Taken together, human brain is a highly complex and dynamic machine, which supports a number of flexible and sophisticated functions including emotion processing and executive functions. Our findings point out a dynamic brain network model, which will shed a light on understanding how complex dynamic brain systems empower flexible cognitive functions.
Catecholamine (CA) neurotransmitters (e.g., dopamine) have long been implicated playing a critical role in executive control and decision making. Recently, evidence from neurodegenerative patients and healthy population suggested that CA also modulates language processing. However, the question of what kind of influence the CA might exert on language is still open. My recent study investigated this question by examining the effects of methylphenidate (MPH), which is a CA agonist, on EEG response related to language processing. The results demonstrated a clear neuropharmacological effect that CA neurotransmitters provide a relevance signal that enhances semantic integration operations, regardless of whether language processing is goal-relevant or not. The Basal Ganglia projections to Frontal Cortex might mediate the language processing effects of these neuropharmacological agents. These results contradicted the general hypothesis that CA promotes goal-relevant processes, while inhibiting goal-irrelevant information, suggesting that our capacity for language is a core system rooted in our biological system.
Bullying-victimization by classroom peers is a common and significant stressor for adolescents and increases their risk of developing adjustment problems, especially depressive symptoms. Some studies have found victims’ social surrounding may modify the association between relational victimization and depression. However, contradictory proposes might be inferred from different theories in terms of influences of prosocial beahvior in classroom context. According to the social support theory, a friendly and prosocial environment could offer more opportunities of making social ties and compensate the interpersonal connection deprived by relational victimization. In contrast, the social comparison theory suggests an opposite effect, that prosocial context might aggravate the adverse effect of stressful interpersonal experience.
The present study examined the two competitive theoretical suggestions about the effect of a prosocial classroom context. More specifically, it investigated the role of classroom prosociality in the developmental association between children’s relational victimization experiences and their depressive symptoms. We used a short-term longitudinal framework, by following 327 pre-adolescents (45.6% boys; Mage = 10.23, SDage = 0.49 at baseline) for one and a half years with 6-month intervals from grade five to grade six. A multilevel (individual level and class level) cross-lagged model was used with classroom prosociality as the moderator on individual level cross-lagged paths from relational victimization to depressive symptoms.
Our results supported the social comparison theory. We found that classroom prosociality aggregated the adverse effect of relational victimization. pecifically, in classes with a high level of prosociality, relationally victimized adolescents were likely to develop depressive symptoms. On the contrary, in classes with a low level of prosociality, the relationally victimized adolescents were less likely to develop depressive symptoms. Our findings suggest that the positive classroom context provides a contrasting background for personal stressful social experience, instead of social support.
何津 Jin He PhD Student, Vrije University Amsterdam
TBD
Humans are constantly bombarded by streams of sensory information. Selective attention determines what we attend and what ignore. Traditionally, it was thought that what we attend was determined by our top-down goals and the physical characteristics of the environment. Recently however, it was pointed out that this conceptualisation may be incomplete as selection is likely to be the result our experiences, i.e., the history of attentional deployments can elicit lingering selection biases, unrelated to top-down goals or the physical salience of items. Invariant properties of the visual environment can bias attention such that we are able to interact more effectively with the visual world. Previous studies have shown that the efficiency of selecting the target can be improved when the target consistently appears at specific locations in the display (e.g., contextual cueing). This is not surprising as the target is relevant for the task and it is known that attention can be directed to locations in space. In this talk, however, I will specifically focus on how statistical regularities regarding items that are not relevant for the task (i.e., distractors) affect selection. We show that through statistical learning, locations that are likely to contain distractors are suppressed relative to all other locations. These statistical regularities drive selection above and beyond top-down and bottom-up control and operate without people being aware of them. We argue that statistical learning induces plasticity in time and place within the spatial priority map such that locations that are likely to contain a target are boosted and locations that are likely to contain a distractor are suppressed. It reflects our ability to extract the distributional properties of sensory input across time and space; one of the most crucial human abilities that plays a role in essentially everything we do.
Action goals in psychological studies are often confused with the result of a single movement, such as reaching or button pressing. However, hand-object interactions are not restricted to a single movement towards the object, but also using the object to accomplish a subsequent goal. Most of the previous studies only used the entire goal (both immediate grip demand and final task goal) and the coordination of the different parts of the action goal is still unclear. In the present study, the cognitive mechanism of immediate and final action goals in planning grasping movement was investigated with the help of the ERP. 28 Participants were asked to perform either free choice or specified grips and rotate a handle to the given target positions based on the cues. The two cues were shown in different sequences which were consisted by the separately presented cues “how to grip” (immediate) and “where to rotate” (final) in the order of immediate-final or final-immediate. The results indicated that neural activity differed between free and specified actions. Final action goal seems more important than grip posture in the preparation of grasping movement.
于淋 Lin Yu TBD, TBD
TBD
Functional neural circuits in the brain require specific connections and coordinated activity of synapses. When this coordination is disrupted, neurological diseases, such as autism, can develop. The ongoing of structural and functional organization of synapses is crucial for maintaining healthy and functional neural networks. These processes are widely investigated in excitatory synapse and great progress was made in the past decades in improving our understanding of the molecular and physiological mechanisms of synapse plasticity. However, the molecular processes underlying inhibitory synaptic plasticity are not well known.
In this study, we examine how presynaptic plasticity in inhibitory axons is regulated by endocannabinoid signaling. We visualize presynaptic boutons in GFP -labeled axons using two-photon live imaging in organotypic hippocampal slice cultures from GAD65-GFP transgenic mice. We found that transient activation of endocannabinoid type 1 receptor (CB1R) affects inhibitory bouton dynamics, but surprisingly we observed that the effect was different when CB1R was activated by the synthetic agonist WIN 55212-2 or by endogenous ligand 2-AG. Our results suggest that interaction kinetics between ligand and CB1R are important to trigger specific downstream signaling pathways that inhibitory axonal plasticity is regulated by non-canonical endocannabinoid signaling
Jian Liang PhD Student, Utrecht University
TBD
Belief-updating is important to make appropriate decisions. However, the usage of prior knowledge and current information for updating might be altered by many factors, such as quantity of current information and emotional state. In this talk, I will focus on my first study that investigated how various quantities of current information is integrated with prior knowledge. To address this issue, the reward information is associated with the probability of occurrence and the size of current information was systematically manipulated trial-by-trial. At behavioral level, we found subjects flexibly adjusted the weights assigned to prior knowledge and current information as predicted by the Bayesian model. At neural level, fMRI results indicated medial prefrontal cortex (mPFC) involved in information integration. Interestingly, the activation in mPFC also represented the relative weights assignment for these two sources of information (Ting et al., 2015).
丁致中 ChihChung Ting TBD, University van Amsterdam
anxiety & reinforcement learning
BACKGROUND
Considerable heterogeneity of dementia progression in cognition and daily functioning exists both between and within individuals. To date, only a limited number of studies have worked on the trajectories of dementia progression, let alone studying multiple outcomes simultaneously. Therefore, this study aimed at replicating latent classes of both cognitive and functional progression in dementia.
METHODS
We included 331 subjects from the Dutch Clinical Course of Cognition and Comorbidity (4C) study, and 1628 subjects from American National Alzheimer’s Coordinating Center in our study. All subjects were incident dementia cases and both cohorts were analyzed separately. Cognitive and functional decline was modelled jointly using a parallel-process growth mixture model.
RESULTS
In each cohort, 3 classes of trajectories for cognition and daily functioning were identified. 30% of the participants from the Dutch cohort and 26% from the American cohort showed slow and stable decline in both cognition and daily functioning. The largest group in both cohorts (44% in the Dutch cohort and 67% in the American cohort) exhibited a slightly faster cognitive decline and poor daily functioning at diagnosis as well as a rapid decline. The remaining patients showed rapid decline in both cognition and daily functioning; 26% of the participants in the Dutch cohort and 7% of the American cohort. The decline of cognition and functioning exhibited a low correlation in both cohorts.
CONCLUSIONS
This study identified 3 classes with similar trajectories of cognitive and functional decline in two different cohorts. A discrepancy between cognitive and functional decline was found in both cohorts, highlighting the importance of considering both domains in clinical practice as well as future research.
王雨薇 Yuwei Wang Master Student, Radboud Institute for Health Sciences
TBD
This study aims at examining the relations between children's shyness in toddlerhood and their empathy and prosocial behavior at preschool age. Specifically, the family environment and family setting in early years are highlighted and tested as predictors and moderators of those developmental relations. Data from a 7-wave longitudinal study (Beijing Longitudinal Study 2010) were analyzed and reported. Children's shyness is composed of mother-reported measurements at 14 (W3) and 25 (W4) months and the observational assessment at 38 (W5) months. Empathy and prosocial behavior were coded from two "pretend-to-hurt" paradigms at 60 months (W6). In addition, mothers reported maternal time and paternal time from 9 (W2) to 60 (W6) months, which are used as an indicator of family setting in early years. Besides, mothers rated family chaos on the subscale of family organization in HOME at 6 (W1) and 25 (W4) months, which are used as the indicator of family environment in toddlerhood. Mothers also rated family environment on CHAOS questionnaire at 38 (W5) and 60 (W6) months, which are tested for its role as family environment in preschool years in children's development of empathy and prosocial behavior. In this study, we focus on the following research questions: 1) How does young children's shyness relate to their empathy and prosocial behavior later at preschool age? 2) How does family setting (indexed by maternal time and paternal time) relate to preschoolers' empathy and prosocial behavior? 3) How does family environment relate to preschoolers' empathy and prosocial behavior? 4) Do person (shyness) x environment (family setting and family environment) transactions predict preschoolers' empathy and prosocial behavior?
Shuyang Dong PhD Student, Utrecht University
TBD
Descritpion TBD.
于淋 Lin Yu TBD, TBD
TBD
丁致中 ChihChung Ting TBD, University van Amsterdam
anxiety & reinforcement learning